Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations

Inhibited children become anxious adults: Examining the causes and effects of early shyness

Date:
April 17, 2014
Source:
Penn State
Summary:
Three little girls sit together in a room, playing with the toys surrounding them. One of the girls -- "Emma" -- has clearly taken charge of the group, and the others happily go along with her. A fourth girl -- "Jane" -- enters the room, hiding her face while clinging to her mother. The first three continue to play, while mom sits Jane down with some toys a few feet away from the group. After mom leaves, however, Jane sits alone against the wall. Emma makes her way over to Jane, inviting her to play with the rest of the group. Jane -- looking trapped -- starts to cry, then stands up and tries desperately to open the door.

Children with behavioral inhibition and shyness show increased risk for social anxiety.
Credit: © Monkey Business / Fotolia

Three little girls sit together in a room, playing with the toys surrounding them. One of the girls -- "Emma" -- has clearly taken charge of the group, and the others happily go along with her. A fourth girl -- "Jane" -- enters the room, hiding her face while clinging to her mother. The first three continue to play, while mom sits Jane down with some toys a few feet away from the group. After mom leaves, however, Jane sits alone against the wall. Emma makes her way over to Jane, inviting her to play with the rest of the group. Jane -- looking trapped -- starts to cry, then stands up and tries desperately to open the door.

This particular scene played out about 15 years ago in the lab where developmental psychologist Koraly Pérez-Edgar was a postdoctoral fellow. The girls, all 4 years old, were videotaped so that the researchers could study their interactions. The differing temperaments of the children are so obvious that Pérez-Edgar, now an associate professor of psychology at Penn State, still shows this video to her graduate students as an example of behavioral inhibition.

"The inhibited child will sit and watch, but she doesn't play alone or with others. The idea of being included appears to terrify her," Pérez-Edgar notes. Her work over the years has demonstrated that this kind of extreme shyness observed in children is often a predictor of anxiety later in life. She says that the way shy children behave will evolve as they grow up, "but they can remain uncomfortable in their own skin in new social situations."

Pérez-Edgar runs the University's Cognition, Affect, and Temperament Lab. She has a team of nearly 30 people working with her: one postdoctoral fellow, three research assistants, five graduate students, and about 20 undergraduates. The researchers work with children from 4 months old through young adulthood, using a variety of tools to aid their work in exploring anxiety. From clinical interviews and questionnaires to genetics and neuroimaging, Pérez-Edgar and company are trying to learn more about what their young subjects are experiencing, why, and how to help.

The beginnings of anxiety

It's rare for a child to be clinically diagnosed with an anxiety disorder prior to adolescence. "Kids aren't yet anxious, but can have the temperament that may predispose them to become anxious," is the way Pérez-Edgar puts it. She is careful to draw a distinction between normal separation anxiety, which is commonly experienced by 2- and 3-year-olds, and what might be called an anxious temperament.

"When [a behaviorally inhibited] baby is exposed to novel sensory information -- it can be something as benign as one of those mobiles you put over the crib or a normal jack-in-the-box -- a lot of babies giggle and laugh, they think it's funny. But these babies are terrified, they cry and arch their backs -- their systems have just said 'danger, danger, danger,'" she explains. Later predictors may include difficulty building relationships and in socializing with peers, as demonstrated in the video.

Developmental psychologist Jerome Kagan, Pérez-Edgar's dissertation advisor at Harvard, was the first to identify behavioral inhibition in children as a potential indicator of anxiety to come. In 1984, Kagan and his student Cynthia Garcia Coll, who later became a professor at Brown University, co-authored two papers discussing behavioral inhibition in 21-month-old toddlers and in 4-year-old children. This is the first time the term "behavioral inhibition" appears in the literature.

Kagan and Garcia Coll looked at the responses of their young subjects to an unfamiliar person or experience: "Some [children] become quiet, cease the activity in which they are engaged, retreat to a familiar person, or withdraw from the field in which the unfamiliar event occurred," they write. The children who responded this way were considered behaviorally inhibited.

When Pérez-Edgar arrived at Harvard, she continued this line of research, working with Kagan and then with an earlier protege of Kagan's, Nathan Fox. By following subjects into their teens and beyond, the researchers were able to confirm the link between behavioral inhibition in young children and anxiety later in life.

Fox, a developmental psychologist at the University of Maryland, began studying a group of 153 4-month-olds in 1993 and 1994. He and Pérez-Edgar, with colleagues at the University of Maryland, have been following this group ever since, observing each child from 8 to 12 times over the years. The 4-year-old girls in the video Pérez-Edgar still uses were part of this larger group -- and likely still are, now as young adults.

Chicken and egg

Once a behavioral link had been established, researchers began to speculate about the neurology involved. Could extreme shyness be traced to differences in the brain? Kagan, among others, predicted that behaviorally inhibited babies -- and the adults they would become -- might have an overly sensitive limbic system, and in particular an overly sensitive amygdala.

"The limbic system is an early-evolving area of the brain that shapes the individual's response to threat and novelty in the environment," says Pérez-Edgar. "It is very phylogenetically old -- lizards have a limbic system." At the center of this system, she continues, is a small region called the amygdala. "If you look at other animals, when the amygdala responds, the first thing they do is they freeze. And often, after freezing, they withdraw."

The amygdala is the seat of what we know as the fight-or-flight reaction. When stimulated, even after millions of years of evolution, it still triggers your nervous system to freeze and assess the situation (likely quicker than you actually have time to process it), and then tells the rest of your body either to stay put and defend or to head for the hills. It turns out that when the amygdala is overly-sensitive when it determines there is reason for fear, it can cause anxiety.

With advances in technology, specifically the use of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), researchers have been able to actually see this link between brain and temperament. After the babies in Fox and Pérez-Edgar's study became teenagers and were able to undergo MRI scans, she reports, "We were able to show that yes indeed, teenagers who as babies looked so fearful in the face of novelty, in fact their amygdalae did respond more vigorously."

At this point, however, the direction of causation remains to be established. "Here we have a chicken versus egg situation," says Pérez-Edgar. "Is it because you're temperamentally reactive that your amygdala is overactive, or vice versa?"

She is currently conducting a study with children ages 9 to 12 to observe how attention and temperament might be linked to social behavior -- that is, the ways a child reacts to social situations. As she points out, the amygdala is not solely activated by fear; it is also known to be responsive to other social stimuli. Over the course of this study Perez-Edgar will observe patterns of social behavior, both by physically watching children interacting and also by collecting EEG (electroencephalogram) and fMRI information.

One way that Pérez-Edgar and colleagues are trying to help anxious children is by behavioral therapy: diverting their attention away from what is making them anxious. The researchers hypothesize that by training the brain of a child to not seek out things that make her anxious and possibly by focusing her attention elsewhere, her anxiety will be reduced.

With this research and more, Pérez-Edgar is hopeful that she and others will be able to gain further insights about how brain structure and behavior influence each other, so that some day Jane can comfortably play with other children and grow into a happy, well-adjusted adult.

Video: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=gStv-LqzsyI


Story Source:

The above story is based on materials provided by Penn State. The original article was written by Victoria M. Indivero. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.


Cite This Page:

Penn State. "Inhibited children become anxious adults: Examining the causes and effects of early shyness." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 17 April 2014. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/04/140417155901.htm>.
Penn State. (2014, April 17). Inhibited children become anxious adults: Examining the causes and effects of early shyness. ScienceDaily. Retrieved July 26, 2014 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/04/140417155901.htm
Penn State. "Inhibited children become anxious adults: Examining the causes and effects of early shyness." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/04/140417155901.htm (accessed July 26, 2014).

Share This




More Mind & Brain News

Saturday, July 26, 2014

Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations


Featured Videos

from AP, Reuters, AFP, and other news services

University Quiz Implies Atheists Are Smarter Than Christians

University Quiz Implies Atheists Are Smarter Than Christians

Newsy (July 25, 2014) — An online quiz from a required course at Ohio State is making waves for suggesting atheists are inherently smarter than Christians. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com
Beatings and Addiction: Pakistan Drug 'clinic' Tortures Patients

Beatings and Addiction: Pakistan Drug 'clinic' Tortures Patients

AFP (July 24, 2014) — A so-called drugs rehab 'clinic' is closed down in Pakistan after police find scores of ‘patients’ chained up alleging serial abuse. Duration 03:05 Video provided by AFP
Powered by NewsLook.com
New Painkiller Designed To Discourage Abuse: Will It Work?

New Painkiller Designed To Discourage Abuse: Will It Work?

Newsy (July 24, 2014) — The FDA approved Targiniq ER on Wednesday, a painkiller designed to keep users from abusing it. Like any new medication, however, it has doubters. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com
Can Watching TV Make You Feel Like A Failure?

Can Watching TV Make You Feel Like A Failure?

Newsy (July 24, 2014) — A study by German researchers claims watching TV while you're stressed out can make you feel guilty and like a failure. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com

Search ScienceDaily

Number of stories in archives: 140,361

Find with keyword(s):
 
Enter a keyword or phrase to search ScienceDaily for related topics and research stories.

Save/Print:
Share:  

Breaking News:
from the past week

In Other News

... from NewsDaily.com

Science News

Health News

Environment News

Technology News



Save/Print:
Share:  

Free Subscriptions


Get the latest science news with ScienceDaily's free email newsletters, updated daily and weekly. Or view hourly updated newsfeeds in your RSS reader:

Get Social & Mobile


Keep up to date with the latest news from ScienceDaily via social networks and mobile apps:

Have Feedback?


Tell us what you think of ScienceDaily -- we welcome both positive and negative comments. Have any problems using the site? Questions?
Mobile iPhone Android Web
Follow Facebook Twitter Google+
Subscribe RSS Feeds Email Newsletters
Latest Headlines Health & Medicine Mind & Brain Space & Time Matter & Energy Computers & Math Plants & Animals Earth & Climate Fossils & Ruins