Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations

Infants May Offer Clues To Language Development

Date:
February 18, 2003
Source:
University Of Wisconsin-Madison
Summary:
By listening to the talk around them, infants pick up sound patterns that help them understand the speech they hear, according to new research from the University of Wisconsin-Madison. But this research also shows that some patterns are easier to identify, suggesting that the development of human language may have been shaped by what infants could learn.

DENVER - You may not know it, but you took a course in linguistics as a baby.

By listening to the talk around them, infants pick up sound patterns that help them understand the speech they hear, according to new research from the University of Wisconsin-Madison. But this research also shows that some patterns are easier to identify, suggesting that the development of human language may have been shaped by what infants could learn.

These results were presented here today, Monday, Feb. 17, at the annual meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science.

In a series of forthcoming papers, psychologist Jenny Saffran, who directs the Infant Learning Laboratory at UW-Madison, suggests how infants quickly acquire language, specifically their ability to find word boundaries - where words begin and end - from a steady stream of speech. "We've known for a long time that babies acquire language rapidly," she says, "but what we haven't known is how they do it."

In all her studies, Saffran introduces her infant listeners to an artificial, or nonsense, language. Examples of words include "giku," "tuka" and "bugo." By using these made-up words, which the tiny listeners have never heard before, Saffran can isolate particular elements found in natural languages such as English.

For just a couple of minutes, the infants hear dozens of two-syllable words strung together in a stream of monotone speech, unbroken by any pauses (for example, gikutukabugo...). The words are presented in a particular order that reveals a sound pattern. If babies recognize the pattern, says Saffran, they will use it to quickly identify word boundaries in what they hear next.

To test this, Saffran introduces her listeners to a new string of nonsense words in which only some of them fit the pattern heard earlier. Saffran records how long the infants listen to the parts that conform to the pattern and the parts that don't. A significant difference in times, she explains, means the infants did pick up the pattern.

As her recent studies show, infants do learn sound patterns, which then help them learn words and, ultimately, grammar. Their ability to do this, however, depends on age.

By exposing infants who are 6-and-a-half and 9 months old to a string of made-up words in a certain order, Saffran learned that the two age groups use different strategies to determine where words end and begin. While the younger listeners identified word boundaries by relying on the likelihood that certain sounds occur together, the older listeners paid attention to what speech sounds were emphasized, or stressed. Because 90 percent of two-syllable words in English follow the same stress pattern, says Saffran, infants can use the pattern to determine the word boundaries.

"At different points in development, babies orient towards some cues and not others," says Saffran. Why? "More linguistic experience." Before infants can recognize that stressed and unstressed syllables are reliable indicators of word boundaries, explains Saffran, they must first know a few words - lessons they learn earlier by learning which sounds are likely to occur together.

Findings from this study will be published in an upcoming issue of the journal Developmental Psychology.

Once infants go from syllables to words, they then can recognize simple grammars, according to Saffran's second study now in press at the journal Infancy. At age one year - just three months after babies begin using stress cues - infants can recognize patterns in word orderings. After listening to a continuous string of words in a particular order, the infants were able to identify permissible word orderings. Just as noted in the other study, Saffran says that only after prior learning can infants acquire additional language abilities: "Until they learn words, the grammar is invisible."

While these two studies looked at babies' ability to acquire sound patterns common in natural languages, a recent third study by the Wisconsin psychologist investigated infants' ability to acquire patterns not often heard in everyday speech. The question Saffran wanted to answer, she says, was, "'Does language work in a way that best fits the brain?'" In other words: Are certain sound patterns more common than others because they make it easier for infants to learn language? This study is in press at Developmental Psychology.

Unlike the other studies, which exposed infants to generalizations in language patterns, such as the grouping of sounds, this study tested an infant's ability to recognize something more specific - that syllables begin with some sounds, such as /p/, /d/ and /k/, but not others, such as /b/, /t/ and /g/. This pattern, says Saffran, is uncommon in phonological systems, which tend to place restrictions on types of sound segments, not individual ones.

As Saffran found when she measured how long the infants listened to words that did and didn't conform to the rare pattern, there was no significant difference in the listening times. This finding, she says, suggests that babies had difficulty acquiring the pattern.

The infants' difficulty in identifying the unusual sound pattern in this third study, she says, is likely to be the result of removing information helpful to young listeners as they acquire language. "There are certain types of patterns that they're better at picking up," adds Saffran. "Perhaps human languages have these patterns to make language more learnable. "

Asking questions about what an infant can't learn, she says, can be just as interesting and informative as asking ones about what they can learn. In addition to providing knowledge about language deficits in some children, the answers could offer clues to how human language first developed and how it has evolved.


Story Source:

The above story is based on materials provided by University Of Wisconsin-Madison. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.


Cite This Page:

University Of Wisconsin-Madison. "Infants May Offer Clues To Language Development." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 18 February 2003. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2003/02/030218085720.htm>.
University Of Wisconsin-Madison. (2003, February 18). Infants May Offer Clues To Language Development. ScienceDaily. Retrieved July 28, 2014 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2003/02/030218085720.htm
University Of Wisconsin-Madison. "Infants May Offer Clues To Language Development." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2003/02/030218085720.htm (accessed July 28, 2014).

Share This




More Mind & Brain News

Monday, July 28, 2014

Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations


Featured Videos

from AP, Reuters, AFP, and other news services

Losing Sleep Leaves You Vulnerable To 'False Memories'

Losing Sleep Leaves You Vulnerable To 'False Memories'

Newsy (July 27, 2014) A new study shows sleep deprivation can make it harder for people to remember specific details of an event. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com
University Quiz Implies Atheists Are Smarter Than Christians

University Quiz Implies Atheists Are Smarter Than Christians

Newsy (July 25, 2014) An online quiz from a required course at Ohio State is making waves for suggesting atheists are inherently smarter than Christians. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com
Beatings and Addiction: Pakistan Drug 'clinic' Tortures Patients

Beatings and Addiction: Pakistan Drug 'clinic' Tortures Patients

AFP (July 24, 2014) A so-called drugs rehab 'clinic' is closed down in Pakistan after police find scores of ‘patients’ chained up alleging serial abuse. Duration 03:05 Video provided by AFP
Powered by NewsLook.com
New Painkiller Designed To Discourage Abuse: Will It Work?

New Painkiller Designed To Discourage Abuse: Will It Work?

Newsy (July 24, 2014) The FDA approved Targiniq ER on Wednesday, a painkiller designed to keep users from abusing it. Like any new medication, however, it has doubters. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com

Search ScienceDaily

Number of stories in archives: 140,361

Find with keyword(s):
Enter a keyword or phrase to search ScienceDaily for related topics and research stories.

Save/Print:
Share:

Breaking News:
from the past week

In Other News

... from NewsDaily.com

Science News

Health News

Environment News

Technology News



Save/Print:
Share:

Free Subscriptions


Get the latest science news with ScienceDaily's free email newsletters, updated daily and weekly. Or view hourly updated newsfeeds in your RSS reader:

Get Social & Mobile


Keep up to date with the latest news from ScienceDaily via social networks and mobile apps:

Have Feedback?


Tell us what you think of ScienceDaily -- we welcome both positive and negative comments. Have any problems using the site? Questions?
Mobile: iPhone Android Web
Follow: Facebook Twitter Google+
Subscribe: RSS Feeds Email Newsletters
Latest Headlines Health & Medicine Mind & Brain Space & Time Matter & Energy Computers & Math Plants & Animals Earth & Climate Fossils & Ruins