Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations

More Complicated Mental Tasks Interfere With Drivers' Ability To Detect Visual Targets

Date:
July 1, 2003
Source:
American Psychological Association
Summary:
Undertaking complex mental tasks can reduce a driver's ability to detect visual targets by as much as 30 percent. New research on driving in real traffic confirms that mental workload can interfere with the capacity to detect visual targets, discriminate among them, and select a response.

Washington -- Undertaking complex mental tasks can reduce a driver's ability to detect visual targets by as much as 30 percent. New research on driving in real traffic confirms that mental workload can interfere with the capacity to detect visual targets, discriminate among them, and select a response. Higher-level mental tasks take attentional resources away from the road, resulting in those all-too-familiar post-accident reports: "I didn't expect it" or "I saw it too late." These findings appear in the June issue of the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied.

Already knowing that external distractions divert drivers, the authors – at the Dirección General De Tráfico, Spain's Public Administration for Traffic Safety and the Universidad Complutense in Madrid -- studied internal distractions produced by the driver's own thoughts or cognitive activity unrelated to the task of driving. "It is easy to understand how one cannot see because of not looking," they explain, "but it is less obvious to explain how one looks but does not see."

The two-psychologist team studied 12 adults who drove for about four hours on the highway north from Madrid. They used a standard Citröen with an unobtrusive eye-tracking system that allowed them to study gaze and ocular fixation for signs of attention and distraction as drivers performed certain mental tasks. Drivers were additionally tested by an automatic system that periodically flashed spotlights in the driver's visual field, to which drivers responded using buttons placed ergonomically near the steering wheel. The researchers analyzed how drivers scanned the road scene, including use of the speedometer and mirrors. Researchers also measured how often the drivers glanced at the flashing spotlights, to identify them before they responded.

The effects of the performance of several mental tasks were compared with the effects of ordinary driving (the control condition). After being asked to attend and keep the information in mind, drivers listened to recorded audio messages with either abstract or concrete information (acquisition task). Next, drivers had to freely generate a reproduction of what they had just listened to (production task).

Although the more receptive tasks – listening and learning -- had little or no effect on performance, there were significant differences in almost all of the measures of attention when drivers had to reproduce the content of the audio message they had just heard.

Drivers also performed other tasks, either live or by phone. One was mental calculus (mentally changing between Euros and Spanish pesetas) either with an experimenter in the car, talking to the driver, or with the driver speaking by hands-free phone. One was a memory task (giving detailed information about where they were and what they were doing at a given day and time). Both tasks produced remarkable distraction effects.

"When performing complex mental tasks," the authors say, "the percentages of detected targets and/or correct responses decreased significantly." Drivers glanced at the targets less frequently, and gave a higher percentage of responses without directly looking in their direction. Also during these tasks, the targets (if looked at) were detected later and glanced at for less time, which would cause poorer identification.

Say the authors, "Some tasks showed a reduction in detection probability of almost 30 percent with respect to the control condition, something that is practically meaningful as an estimate of the increased risk of distraction errors hypothetically leading to traffic conflicts or accidents."

Driver errors appeared to derive from deficient target perception and/or identification rather then from a problem with decision rules and/or response performance. In other words, mentally distracted drivers still know how to drive, but don't see things well or fast enough to safely use their skills.

In the experimental variation that examined the impact of hands-free phone conversation, message complexity made the difference. The relative safety of low-demand phone conversation -- if hands-free and voice-operated --appeared to be about the same as that of live conversation. Thus, the authors say that because they demand too much attention, "Complex conversations, whether by phone or with a passenger, are dangerous for road safety." The participants only answered phone calls; the study did not include dialing.

The authors urge further study to assess the impact of another result, the spatial gaze concentration accompanied by a marked reduction in the speedometer and rearview mirror inspection, which "can be interpreted as a reduced situation awareness but also as an example of a balance to optimize visual resources."

The results are important, the authors conclude, for "the evaluation of the potential impact of in-car devices, the improvement of intelligent vehicle-user interfaces, and the issue of how to present information to drivers with minimal interference."

The findings also confirm that the risk of endogenous distraction (internal to the driver, as in complex thought) is at least as relevant as exogenous distraction (external, as in a crying child in the back seat or an accident scene on the road).

The authors stress that because distraction cannot be directly submitted to surveillance and enforcement, safety measures "such as removing potential external distractors or restricting the use of in-vehicle devices, could be effective if users and legislators feel committed to the importance of attentional control for road safety." Without such commitment, they conclude, drivers will again adapt and underestimate the importance of attention and the risk of distraction, including their own mental activity while driving.

###

Article: "Mental Workload While Driving: Effects on Visual Search, Discrimination and Decision Making," Miguel Angel Recarte Goldarecena, Ph.D., Universidad Complutense, and Luis Miguel Nunes González, Ph.D., Dirección General de Tráfico; Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, Vol. 9, No. 2.


Story Source:

The above story is based on materials provided by American Psychological Association. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.


Cite This Page:

American Psychological Association. "More Complicated Mental Tasks Interfere With Drivers' Ability To Detect Visual Targets." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 1 July 2003. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2003/06/030630110251.htm>.
American Psychological Association. (2003, July 1). More Complicated Mental Tasks Interfere With Drivers' Ability To Detect Visual Targets. ScienceDaily. Retrieved April 20, 2014 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2003/06/030630110251.htm
American Psychological Association. "More Complicated Mental Tasks Interfere With Drivers' Ability To Detect Visual Targets." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2003/06/030630110251.htm (accessed April 20, 2014).

Share This



More Mind & Brain News

Sunday, April 20, 2014

Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations


Featured Videos

from AP, Reuters, AFP, and other news services

Study On Artists' Brain Shows They're 'Structurally Unique'

Study On Artists' Brain Shows They're 'Structurally Unique'

Newsy (Apr. 17, 2014) — The brains of artists aren't really left-brain or right-brain, but rather have extra neural matter in visual and motor control areas. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com
Is Apathy A Sign Of A Shrinking Brain?

Is Apathy A Sign Of A Shrinking Brain?

Newsy (Apr. 17, 2014) — A recent study links apathetic feelings to a smaller brain. Researchers say the results indicate a need for apathy screening for at-risk seniors. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com
Are School Dress Codes Too Strict?

Are School Dress Codes Too Strict?

AP (Apr. 16, 2014) — Pushing the limits on style and self-expression is a rite of passage for teens and even younger kids. How far should schools go with their dress codes? The courts have sided with schools in an era when school safety is paramount. (April 16) Video provided by AP
Powered by NewsLook.com
Could Even Casual Marijuana Use Alter Your Brain?

Could Even Casual Marijuana Use Alter Your Brain?

Newsy (Apr. 16, 2014) — A new study conducted by researchers at Northwestern and Harvard suggests even casual marijuana use can alter your brain. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com

Search ScienceDaily

Number of stories in archives: 140,361

Find with keyword(s):
 
Enter a keyword or phrase to search ScienceDaily for related topics and research stories.

Save/Print:
Share:  

Breaking News:
from the past week

In Other News

... from NewsDaily.com

Science News

Health News

Environment News

Technology News



Save/Print:
Share:  

Free Subscriptions


Get the latest science news with ScienceDaily's free email newsletters, updated daily and weekly. Or view hourly updated newsfeeds in your RSS reader:

Get Social & Mobile


Keep up to date with the latest news from ScienceDaily via social networks and mobile apps:

Have Feedback?


Tell us what you think of ScienceDaily -- we welcome both positive and negative comments. Have any problems using the site? Questions?
Mobile iPhone Android Web
Follow Facebook Twitter Google+
Subscribe RSS Feeds Email Newsletters
Latest Headlines Health & Medicine Mind & Brain Space & Time Matter & Energy Computers & Math Plants & Animals Earth & Climate Fossils & Ruins