New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Wrongful convictions can be reduced through science, but tradeoffs exist

Date:
May 22, 2012
Source:
Association for Psychological Science
Summary:
Many of the wrongful convictions identified in a report this week hinged on a misidentified culprit -- now, scientific research reveals the paradox of reforms in eyewitness identification procedure. In our efforts to ensure good guys don't get locked up, we could let more bad guys go. Scholars in psychology and law debate aspects of eyewitness identification procedures, providing a scientific foundation for this important social issue.
Share:
FULL STORY

Many of the wrongful convictions identified in a report this week hinged on a misidentified culprit -- and a new report in a top journal on psychological science reveals the paradox of reforms in eyewitness identification procedure. In our efforts to make sure that good guys don't get locked up, we could let more bad guys go.

In the May issue of Perspectives on Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science, leading scholars in psychology and the law explore and debate various aspects of eyewitness identification procedures, providing a scientific foundation for this important social issue.

In recent years, researchers and policymakers have called for specific reforms to eyewitness identification procedures that would help to reduce the rate of false identification. These reforms affect various aspects of identification procedure, including how lineups are constructed, what witnesses are told prior to the lineup, and how police officers should behave during the procedure.

Such procedural reforms are often viewed as having 'no cost' -- they reduce the false identification rate without affecting the correct identification rate. But psychological scientist Steven Clark (University of California, Riverside) argues that 'no cost' view may not actually be true. After extensive review of the existing data, Clark points out the paradoxical tradeoffs to reforms in eyewitness identification procedure. Existing data suggest that when we choose to enact reforms that are designed to reduce false identifications, we may also reduce the number of correct identifications at the same time.

But this tradeoff does not tell the whole story, say Gary Wells (Iowa State University), Nancy Steblay (Augsburg College), and Jennifer Dysart (John Jay College of Criminal Justice). While reform procedures may reduce the number of 'hits,' they do so by minimizing the influence of suggestive and coercive practices, such as biased instructions and cues from lineup administrators. Wells and his co-authors argue that the so-called 'lost' hits aren't actually relevant, because hits that result from suggestive practices are not legitimate identifications. Eryn Newman from Victoria University of Wellington and Elizabeth Loftus from the University of California, Irvine agree, arguing that eyewitness identification evidence should be based solely on the independent memory of the witness, not on the results of suggestive or coercive procedures.

There is, however, a scientifically valid way to compare witness-identification procedures, say John Wixted and Laura Mickes, both of the University of California, San Diego. If we identify the procedures that reliably differentiate between innocent and guilty suspects over time and across different situations, we will be able to determine which techniques are diagnostically superior to others.

Until we have such comprehensive data, the best way to protect innocent defendants, says Larry Laudan of the University of Texas, is by clearly communicating the fact that eyewitness identifications, regardless of their format, are fallible. According to Laudan, we now have enough empirical data to be able to inform jurors about the error profiles of various eyewitness identification procedures. Sharing this information, he argues, is more important than trying to arrive at "the one unique and definitive format for conducting identifications."

In the end, Clark points out that the goal of his article is not to argue for or against any particular witness identification procedure. Rather, he hopes to create strong links between social science data and public policy. "To the extent that social science research has a useful role in shaping policy decisions," says Clark, "social scientists must do for policymakers what they do best and what policymakers cannot do for themselves: conduct careful studies, and provide a clear and complete analysis of the empirical data."

  • Steven E. Clark et al. "Costs and Benefits of Eyewitness Identification Reform: Psychological Science and Public Policy," Perspectives on Psychological Science, May 2012
  • Nancy K. Steblay et al., "Eyewitness Identification Reforms: Are Suggestiveness-Induced Hits and Guesses True Hits?" Perspectives on Psychological Science, May 2012
  • Elizabeth F. Loftus et al., "Clarkian Logic on Trial," Perspectives on Psychological Science, May 2012
  • John T. Wixted et al., "The Field of Eyewitness Memory Should Abandon Probative Value and Embrace Receiver Operating Characteristic Analysis," Perspectives on Psychological Science, May 2012
  • Larry Laudan et al. "Eyewitness Identifications: One More Lesson on the Costs of Excluding Relevant Evidence," Perspectives on Psychological Science, May 2012

Story Source:

Materials provided by Association for Psychological Science. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Cite This Page:

Association for Psychological Science. "Wrongful convictions can be reduced through science, but tradeoffs exist." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 22 May 2012. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/05/120522135305.htm>.
Association for Psychological Science. (2012, May 22). Wrongful convictions can be reduced through science, but tradeoffs exist. ScienceDaily. Retrieved April 20, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/05/120522135305.htm
Association for Psychological Science. "Wrongful convictions can be reduced through science, but tradeoffs exist." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/05/120522135305.htm (accessed April 20, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES