Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations

To mark territory or not to mark territory: Breaking the pheromone code

Date:
April 24, 2014
Source:
Scripps Research Institute
Summary:
The surprisingly versatile code by which chemical cues help trigger some of the most basic behaviors in mice has been deciphered by scientists. The findings shed light on the evolution of mammalian behaviors -- which include human behaviors -- and their underlying brain mechanisms. They also challenge the traditional view of how pheromones work in animals such as mice. These compounds have until now been thought to trigger behaviors very directly and simply -- one compound, when detected, triggers one behavior -- so that behaviors critical for survival and reproduction don’t have to be learned.

Yellow necked mouse (stock image). This study centered on an important mouse behavior known as countermarking. In the wild, a male mouse typically marks his territory with splashes of urine.
Credit: © creativenature.nl / Fotolia

A team led by scientists at The Scripps Research Institute (TSRI) has deciphered the surprisingly versatile code by which chemical cues help trigger some of the most basic behaviors in mice.

The findings shed light on the evolution of mammalian behaviors—which include human behaviors—and their underlying brain mechanisms.

“How does an individual respond differently to the environment based on experience? How does it distinguish itself from others? These are some of the fundamental questions that a study like this one helps us address,” said TSRI Associate Professor Lisa Stowers, senior author of the new study, which appears in the April 24, 2014 issue of the journal Cell.

The study centered on an important mouse behavior known as countermarking. In the wild, a male mouse typically marks his territory with splashes of urine. “His purpose is to advertise his presence to females and to scare away subordinate males,” said Stowers. But sometimes the mouse encounters a urine mark made by another mouse. In response, he may leave a countermark—to say, “No, this is my territory.” However, if the urine mark has come from a locally dominant mouse, a subordinate mouse will make no countermark and will simply move on.

The findings challenge the traditional view of how pheromones work in animals such as mice. These compounds have until now been thought to trigger behaviors very directly and simply—one compound, when detected, triggers one behavior—so that behaviors critical for survival and reproduction don’t have to be learned.

Chemical Cues

To understand better the chemical cues that guide countermarking decisions, Stowers and her team, including first author Angeldeep W. Kaur, then a research associate in the Stowers laboratory, examined a special set of 21 molecules in mouse urine. Known as Major Urinary Proteins (MUPs), these suspected signaling molecules are linked to testosterone production and are found at high concentrations in the urine of adult male mice.

MUPs are not odorants detected by the main olfactory system; they are pheromones detected by the vomeronasal organ (VNO) at the front of the mouse nose. Humans apparently don’t have a working VNO, but mice, along with many other mammals, do; and because the VNO serves as a more or less direct trigger of behaviors, it can be a handy and powerful tool for studying such behaviors and the neural logic on which they are based.

A male mouse in the wild expresses his own distinctive combination of several MUPs in different ratios—what has been hypothesized to be a pheromonal “bar code” of identity. By exposing mice—and even their VNO neurons in tissue-slice cultures—to single and combined MUPs, Kaur, Stowers and their colleagues were able to confirm that male mice learn their own self code and can distinguish it from the codes contained in urine marks from other mice.

“When a urine mark contains a set of MUPs that are similar to the mouse’s own repertoire, he won’t respond to it, but if we vary the set of MUPs in the mark, he will countermark,” said Stowers.

The experiments revealed, however, that the mouse won’t countermark a spot of non-self urine if it has previously learned to associate the sensed set of MUPs in that urine with a dominant mouse. This response shows that the VNO is capable of activating behaviors in a manner that is more complex and versatile and less hardwired—and thus is more like other sensory systems found in humans.

“Here it is using what we call combinatorial coding, and at the same time it is dependent on prior learning, and both of those features are also found in the main olfactory system,” said Stowers.

On the whole, the findings indicate that even in the VNO, a small number of cueing molecules, working through a small set of neurons, can flexibly control several distinct behaviors, including behaviors such as rage and self/other discrimination that are also basic elements of the human experience.

“We’re using the VNO as a simple model system here, but the implications of these coding schemes go beyond the VNO—we suspect that they are used generally by the mammalian brain,” Stowers said. “And now that we know these signaling molecules, and the neurons and the behaviors they activate, we can start to follow the circuits into the brain to explore how these behaviors come about.”


Story Source:

The above story is based on materials provided by Scripps Research Institute. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Angeldeep W. Kaur, Tobias Ackels, Tsung-Han Kuo, Annika Cichy, Sandeepa Dey, Cristen Hays, Maria Kateri, Darren W. Logan, Tobias F. Marton, Marc Spehr, Lisa Stowers. Murine Pheromone Proteins Constitute a Context-Dependent Combinatorial Code Governing Multiple Social Behaviors. Cell, 2014; 157 (3): 676 DOI: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.025

Cite This Page:

Scripps Research Institute. "To mark territory or not to mark territory: Breaking the pheromone code." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 24 April 2014. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/04/140424124700.htm>.
Scripps Research Institute. (2014, April 24). To mark territory or not to mark territory: Breaking the pheromone code. ScienceDaily. Retrieved July 25, 2014 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/04/140424124700.htm
Scripps Research Institute. "To mark territory or not to mark territory: Breaking the pheromone code." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2014/04/140424124700.htm (accessed July 25, 2014).

Share This




More Plants & Animals News

Friday, July 25, 2014

Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations


Featured Videos

from AP, Reuters, AFP, and other news services

How to Make Single Serving Smoothies: Howdini Hacks

How to Make Single Serving Smoothies: Howdini Hacks

Howdini (July 24, 2014) — Smoothies are a great way to get in lots of healthy ingredients, plus they taste great! Howdini has a trick for making the perfect single-size smoothie that will save you time on cleanup too! All you need is a blender and a mason jar. Video provided by Howdini
Powered by NewsLook.com
Boy Attacked by Shark in Florida

Boy Attacked by Shark in Florida

Reuters - US Online Video (July 24, 2014) — An 8-year-old boy is bitten in the leg by a shark while vacationing at a Florida beach. Linda So reports. Video provided by Reuters
Powered by NewsLook.com
Goma Cheese Brings Whiff of New Hope to DRC

Goma Cheese Brings Whiff of New Hope to DRC

Reuters - Business Video Online (July 24, 2014) — The eastern region of the Democratic Republic of Congo, mainly known for conflict and instability, is an unlikely place for the production of fine cheese. But a farm in the village of Masisi, in North Kivu is slowly transforming perceptions of the area. Known simply as Goma cheese, the Congolese version of Dutch gouda has gained popularity through out the region. Ciara Sutton reports. Video provided by Reuters
Powered by NewsLook.com
Tyrannosaur Pack-Hunting Theory Aided By New Footprints

Tyrannosaur Pack-Hunting Theory Aided By New Footprints

Newsy (July 24, 2014) — A new study claims a set of prehistoric T-Rex footprints supports the theory that the giant predators hunted in packs instead of alone. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com

Search ScienceDaily

Number of stories in archives: 140,361

Find with keyword(s):
 
Enter a keyword or phrase to search ScienceDaily for related topics and research stories.

Save/Print:
Share:  

Breaking News:
from the past week

In Other News

... from NewsDaily.com

Science News

Health News

Environment News

Technology News



Save/Print:
Share:  

Free Subscriptions


Get the latest science news with ScienceDaily's free email newsletters, updated daily and weekly. Or view hourly updated newsfeeds in your RSS reader:

Get Social & Mobile


Keep up to date with the latest news from ScienceDaily via social networks and mobile apps:

Have Feedback?


Tell us what you think of ScienceDaily -- we welcome both positive and negative comments. Have any problems using the site? Questions?
Mobile iPhone Android Web
Follow Facebook Twitter Google+
Subscribe RSS Feeds Email Newsletters
Latest Headlines Health & Medicine Mind & Brain Space & Time Matter & Energy Computers & Math Plants & Animals Earth & Climate Fossils & Ruins