New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Information is contagious among social connections

Date:
November 16, 2015
Source:
Association for Psychological Science
Summary:
New research using advanced computer modeling sheds light on how behaviors may become 'contagious' in large groups, showing that the memory of one individual can indirectly influence that of another via shared social connections.
Share:
FULL STORY

New research using advanced computer modeling sheds light on how behaviors may become "contagious" in large groups, showing that the memory of one individual can indirectly influence that of another via shared social connections. The findings are published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

"In large social networks, our model demonstrated that information is 'contagious' in much the same way that behavior seems to be contagious," say researchers Christian Luhmann and Suparna Rajaram of Stony Brook University. "These results suggest that information transmission is a critical mechanism underlying the social transmission of behavior."

While research has shown that various behaviors, including smoking, seem to spread throughout social networks, the mechanisms driving this behavioral contagion remain mysterious. To shed light on these contagious phenomena, Luhmann and Rajaram decided to incorporate well-established cognitive processes into computer models capable of simulating groups much larger than those typically seen in laboratory research. In doing so, they would be able to see how individuals interact, and how information flows, within groups that ranged from two to 500 people.

The researchers started by simulating information sharing in small three-person groups. Each simulated group member was programmed to be able to memorize information and learn from the other two members of the group, in accordance with prior findings derived from experiments with real people to reflect actual memory-related processes.

The recall ability of the collaborative group was compared to the combined memory performance of three simulated individuals who studied and recalled words independently. In total, the researchers simulated 1000 group-versus-individual comparisons.

The results directly mirrored patterns of information sharing and collective memory seen among real people. Specifically, the three-person groups were able to recall significantly fewer items than were the three individuals combined together, a phenomenon known as "collaborative inhibition." The findings indicated that this occurs because individuals who learn together in groups tend to converge on similar information, limiting the overall amount of information that can be learned by the group.

Luhmann and Rajaram then expanded their investigation to examine collaborative groups of different sizes, from two to 128 members.

Again, the results echoed those found in small-scale experiments with real people. The researchers found that collaborative inhibition increased as the groups grew from two to seven members -- each additional member improved the overall recall of collaborative groups but these groups continued to lag behind the performance of the same number of individuals working in isolation.

The results suggest that the limitations on information sharing seen in small groups can't be overcome by simply adding more members to the group. As group size increased beyond seven individuals, the collaborative groups starting catching up to the combined individuals, who had already achieved peak performance.

Then, to see how information would spread throughout a large group, Luhmann and Rajaram created a model that placed individuals into large, realistic networks and allowed them to interact with their "neighbors."

As one might expect, the results showed that direct neighbors showed more similar knowledge than did individuals who were indirectly linked through a shared neighbor.

However, the individuals that shared a neighbor, but who never interacted with each other, showed more similar knowledge than those who were separated by two people in the network.

This suggests that a shared neighbor acts as a go-between, transmitting information to the individuals on either side, allowing them to indirectly influence each other. The researchers found that this indirect influence waned as the distance between two individuals grew, leveling off after six degrees of separation.

"We know that social influences on memory are complex, and yet our simplified model is sufficient to account for key findings from previous work, both within small groups and large social networks," say Luhmann and Rajaram. "Taken together, the findings of the current study leverage laboratory results and explanations to develop a much-needed theoretical account of behavior in large social networks."

The researchers believe that these findings could shed light on the mechanisms that drive real-world contagious phenomena, including smoking, obesity, eyewitness testimony, and even fads and fashion.


Story Source:

Materials provided by Association for Psychological Science. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. C. C. Luhmann, S. Rajaram. Memory Transmission in Small Groups and Large Networks: An Agent-Based Model. Psychological Science, 2015; DOI: 10.1177/0956797615605798

Cite This Page:

Association for Psychological Science. "Information is contagious among social connections." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 16 November 2015. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/11/151116084007.htm>.
Association for Psychological Science. (2015, November 16). Information is contagious among social connections. ScienceDaily. Retrieved March 28, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/11/151116084007.htm
Association for Psychological Science. "Information is contagious among social connections." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/11/151116084007.htm (accessed March 28, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES