New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Community size matters when people create a new language

Date:
July 17, 2019
Source:
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
Summary:
Why do some languages have simpler grammars than others? Researchers propose that the size of the community influences the complexity of the language that evolves in it. When small and large groups of participants played a 'communication game' using only gibberish words they had to invent, the languages invented by larger groups were more systematic than languages of smaller groups, showing that community size is important for shaping grammar.
Share:
FULL STORY

Why are languages so different from each other? After comparing more than two thousand languages, scientists noticed that languages with more speakers are usually simpler than smaller languages. For instance, most English nouns can be turned into plurals by simply adding -s, whereas the German system is notoriously irregular. Linguists have proposed that languages adapt to fit different social structures. "But we actually don't know whether it is the size of the community that drives the difference in complexity," says lead author Limor Raviv from the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. Perhaps the bigger languages have simpler grammars because they cover larger geographical space and speakers are far from each other, or because large communities have more contact with outsiders. Together with her colleagues, Antje Meyer from the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics and Radboud University and Shiri Lev-Ari from the Royal Holloway University of London, Raviv set out to test whether community size alone plays a role in shaping grammar.

'Wowo-ik' and 'wowo-ii'

To test the role of group size experimentally, the psycholinguists used a communication game. In this game, participants had to communicate without using any language they know, leading them to create a new language. The goal of the game was to communicate successfully about different novel scenes, using only invented nonsense words. A 'speaker' would see one of four shapes moving in some direction on a screen and type in nonsense words to describe the scene (its shape and direction). The 'listener' would then guess which scene the other person was referring to, by selecting one of eight scenes on their own screen. Participants received points for every successful interaction (correct guesses). Participants paired up with a different person from their group at every new round, taking turns producing and guessing words.

At the start of the game, people would randomly guess meanings and make up new names. Over the course of several hours, participants started to combine words or part-words systematically, creating an actual mini-language. For instance, in one group, 'wowo-ik' meant that a specific shape was going up and right, whereas 'wowo-ii' meant that the same shape was going straight up. With such a 'regular' system, it becomes easier to predict the meaning of new labels ('mop-ik' meant a different shape going up and right). Participants played in either 'small' groups of four participants or 'large' groups of eight participants. Would the large groups invent 'simpler' (more systematic) languages than the small groups?

Variability promotes structure

By the end of the experiment, the large groups had indeed created languages with more systematic grammars. "The pressure to create systematic languages is much higher in larger groups," explains Raviv. This is because there are more word versions in the larger groups. In order to understand each other, members of a large group must overcome this variability and develop systematic structure. So the more variability, the more it pushed people to make their language even simpler. The researchers also found that the size of the group predicted how similar that group was to the other groups. All large groups reached similar levels of structure and communicative success. However, the small groups differed a lot from each other: some never developed systematic grammars, while others were very successful." This might mean that in the real world, larger languages are potentially more similar to each other than smaller languages," Raviv says.

"Our study shows that the social environment in which languages evolve, and specifically the number of people in the community, can affect the grammar of languages," concludes Raviv. "This could possibly explain why some languages have more complex grammar than others. The results also support the idea that an increase in human population size was one of the main drivers for the evolution of natural languages."


Story Source:

Materials provided by Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Limor Raviv, Antje Meyer, Shiri Lev-Ari. Larger communities create more systematic languages. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2019; 286 (1907): 20191262 DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2019.1262

Cite This Page:

Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. "Community size matters when people create a new language." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 17 July 2019. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/07/190717195354.htm>.
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. (2019, July 17). Community size matters when people create a new language. ScienceDaily. Retrieved October 10, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/07/190717195354.htm
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. "Community size matters when people create a new language." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/07/190717195354.htm (accessed October 10, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES