New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

80 percent of Burmese long-tailed macaques use stone-tools to hammer food

Date:
May 13, 2015
Source:
PLOS
Summary:
Eighty percent of a population of Burmese long-tailed macaques on an island in southern Thailand use stone and shell tools to crack open seafood, and do so using 17 different action patterns.
Share:
FULL STORY

Eighty percent of a population of Burmese long-tailed macaques on an island in southern Thailand use stone and shell tools to crack open seafood, and do so using 17 different action patterns, according to a study published May 13, 2015 in the open-access journal PLOS ONE by Amanda Tan from Nanyang Technological University, Singapore, and colleagues, under an 8 year field project led by Michael D Gumert, also from NTU.

The authors of the study explored variation in how Burmese long-tailed macaques used percussive stone and shell tools to hammer coastal foods on Piak Nam Yai and Thao Islands in Laem Son National Park, Thailand. First, they catalogued the parts tool that macaques used for hammering: the flat face, narrow edge, or point. Next, they categorized the action patterns the macaques used during the hammering, including hand use, posture, and striking motion, for over 600 tool-uses across 90 individuals. Once the tool use and action patterns were identified, they observed over 100 macaques in over 3000 time points on Piak Nam Yai Island's coasts, to determine the proportion of individuals using each tool and action pattern.

Analysis of the observation showed that 80% of macaques used tools, supporting past findings from the project, each employing one to four different action patterns, and a total of 17 different action patterns in the population. Most commonly, the macaques used one-handed hammering with the points of smaller tools to crack open sessile rock oysters that required precision striking, and used one- or two-handed hammering with the faces and edges of larger tools to crack unattached shellfish that had to first be placed on anvils, reflecting different techniques for different foods.

The authors suggest that cataloguing the tools and actions involved in macaque tool use lays the foundation for future studies, such as understanding how macaque tool use develops, and comparing macaque tool use with that of other stone-tool-users in the primate lineage.


Story Source:

Materials provided by PLOS. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Amanda Tan, Say Hoon Tan, Dhaval Vyas, Suchinda Malaivijitnond, Michael D. Gumert. There Is More than One Way to Crack an Oyster: Identifying Variation in Burmese Long-Tailed Macaque (Macaca fascicularis aurea) Stone-Tool Use. PLOS ONE, 2015; 10 (5): e0124733 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124733

Cite This Page:

PLOS. "80 percent of Burmese long-tailed macaques use stone-tools to hammer food." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 13 May 2015. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/05/150513145610.htm>.
PLOS. (2015, May 13). 80 percent of Burmese long-tailed macaques use stone-tools to hammer food. ScienceDaily. Retrieved March 28, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/05/150513145610.htm
PLOS. "80 percent of Burmese long-tailed macaques use stone-tools to hammer food." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2015/05/150513145610.htm (accessed March 28, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES