New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Brain's ability to synchronize voice sounds could be related to language learning

Date:
March 4, 2019
Source:
University of Barcelona
Summary:
Researchers studied the synchronization of speech motor rhythms. Results show some people's brains adapt spontaneously to align with the rhythm of the voices they hear, while others do not. According to the research study, these patterns show differences in functional and structural aspects of the speech network in the brain, as well as the ability to understand new words.
Share:
FULL STORY

Researchers from the Institute of Neurosciences of the University of Barcelona (NeuroUB) and the Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute (IDIBELL) and from the New York University (USA), studied the synchronization of speech motor rhythms, i.e. the coordinated movements of the tongue, lips and jaw that build up the speech, with speech audio rhythms. Results show some people's brains adapt spontaneously to align with the rhythm of the voices they hear, while others do not. According to the research study in Nature Neuroscience, these patterns show differences in functional and structural aspects of the speech network in the brain, as well as the ability to understand new words. These findings would help assessing the speech and cognitive development in kids.

The study is led by the ICREA professor Ruth de Diego-Balaguer and David Poeppel, from New York University (USA), counting on the participation of the UB researcher Joan Orpella and other experts from New York University.

Two different patterns

Humans are good at synchronizing body movements with sound, for instance, when we move our feet or the head to the rhythm of a song. This happens without trying, without having trained and it has even been proved in babies. Most of the current research on this field has focused on how body movements are encouraged by the rhythm of the music, but there is little known about how this synchronization works when it comes to speech.

Aiming to focus on the link between motor rhythms and speech audio signals, researchers designed an apparently easy task: for a minute, participants had to listen to a rhythmical sequence ("la" "di" "fum." ..), and at the same time, they had to whisper "tah." The analysis and results showed an unexpected pattern: population is divided into two groups. While some people spontaneously synchronize whispers with that sequence (good synchronizers), others did not experience any effect from the external rhythm (bad synchronizers). "This effect is surprisingly strong and stable over time," says Ruth de Diego Balaguer, researcher at the Cognition and Brain Plasticity research group of IDIBELL and UB. "Actually, -she continues- we reproduced these patterns in more than 300 people under different conditions, for instance, we saw that the same good and bad synchronizers experienced the same if they conducted the same task on the same day, one week later, and one month later."

Differences in neural connections and behaviour

Given such different patterns, researchers studied whether these variations had implications in the brain organization and behaviour. To study physiological differences, they got data from magnetic resonances carried out on participants using a diffusion weighted MRI technique which enables the reconstruction of white matter fibres that connect the different brain regions. Results show that good synchronizers have more white matter in those pathways connecting speech-perception areas (listening) with speech-production areas (speaking).

They also conducted a magnetoencephalography protocol to record the neural activity among participants while these were passively listening to rhythmical sequences. The study shows good synchronizers showed more brain-stimulus alignment than the other group of people and did so in the brain area involved in speech motor planning. "This implies that those speech production-related areas related are also involved in the speech perception, which may help us monitor the rhythm of external voices," note the researcher.

Last, researchers checked differences in the behaviour of both groups. "We tested whether there were differences in learning new words they listened in continuous talk between a good and a bad synchronizer and we saw good synchronizers perform better than the other group," says Ruth de Diego Balaguer.

A methodology opening new ways for research

The experiment in this study could serve to characterize individual differences and promote language research. "This methodology can help finding effects that were hidden by grouping populations with different neural and behaviour attributes. Also, we think the use of this test could reinforce the early diagnostic of some pathologies (such as Alzheimer's, Parkinson's or Multiple Sclerosis) and help assessing the speech and cognitive development in kids," concludes the researcher.


Story Source:

Materials provided by University of Barcelona. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. M. Florencia Assaneo, Pablo Ripollés, Joan Orpella, Wy Ming Lin, Ruth de Diego-Balaguer, David Poeppel. Spontaneous synchronization to speech reveals neural mechanisms facilitating language learning. Nature Neuroscience, 2019; DOI: 10.1038/s41593-019-0353-z

Cite This Page:

University of Barcelona. "Brain's ability to synchronize voice sounds could be related to language learning." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 4 March 2019. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/03/190304121537.htm>.
University of Barcelona. (2019, March 4). Brain's ability to synchronize voice sounds could be related to language learning. ScienceDaily. Retrieved March 28, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/03/190304121537.htm
University of Barcelona. "Brain's ability to synchronize voice sounds could be related to language learning." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/03/190304121537.htm (accessed March 28, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES