Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations

Some Animals Can Recognize Degree Of Kinship By Scent

Date:
March 26, 2002
Source:
Cornell University
Summary:
The tiny Belding's ground squirrels appear to be "kissing". Instead, they are sniffing to analyze secretions from facial scent glands, hoping to learn from the complex odor bouquet who is family and who's not. More remarkably, they are determining in a matter of seconds precisely who is close-enough kin to risk their lives helping -- and perhaps even whether they are too closely related to for mating.

ITHACA, N.Y. -- The tiny Belding's ground squirrels appear to be "kissing". Instead, they are sniffing to analyze secretions from facial scent glands, hoping to learn from the complex odor bouquet who is family and who's not. More remarkably, they are determining in a matter of seconds precisely who is close-enough kin to risk their lives helping -- and perhaps even whether they are too closely related to for mating.

Related Articles


"It's as if these squirrels are reading DNA fingerprints and drawing the family tree with their noses," says Cornell University psychology researcher Jill M. Mateo. Her five years of field studies in the California mountains, as reported in the Proceedings: Biological Sciences (April 7), a journal of The Royal Society, Britain's national academy of science, are the first to show how recognition odors allow precise estimates of kinship, even among distant relatives.

Published as "Kin-recognition abilities and nepotism as a function of sociality" and funded by the National Science Foundation, the study examines potentially risky nepotistic behaviors that are shown to close relatives. It aims to answer the questions: How do social animals, such as Belding's ground squirrels, determine which other animals have a genome so similar to theirs that helping them survive is like perpetuating one's own complement of genes? And how do they know where to draw the line, declining to help more distant relatives because, even though they smell vaguely similar, the genetic benefits of helping do not outweigh the costs of help?

The psychologist is reluctant to extend the squirrel behavioral studies to human affairs. Yet she is mindful of experiments by others, including studies of human odors, which suggest that kin-recognition by smell evolved across a wide variety of animals. So far, Belding's ground squirrels are the champions of kin-sniffing, but many more species remain to be investigated.

"The word nepotism originally meant favoritism shown to nephews, but that's further than these ground squirrels will go. Nepotism in Belding's ground squirrels is limited to mothers, sisters and daughters. Nieces, nephews and more distant kin can be sensed in considerable detail but they are treated like outsiders," Mateo says. She describes the risks and benefits of nepotism in a squirrel world where territorial defense and predator-avoidance often spell the difference between finding the next meal or becoming one:

o Because male Belding's squirrels leave their natal territory after weaning, colonies of burrows are inhabited by related females of various ages (and a few young males that have yet to leave).

o Cooperative territorial defense is important to preserve nest sites and to keep infanticidal adults away from vulnerable young, but fighting can cost injuries or even lives. Risking life and limb for distant kin or unrelated animals doesn't make evolutionary sense.

o Sounding the alarm when a predator approaches is even more risky, making a helpful squirrel the first target of a coyote, for example, while the others run for cover. Belding's ground squirrels are more likely to give the alarm if they can help close kin (even if they are eaten, nearly identical copies of their genes are hiding in nearby holes) but they need a precise measurement of relatedness to weigh the benefits of risky behavior.

Previously published studies by Mateo and another Cornell psychologist, Robert E. Johnston, had shown how hamsters use their abilities for "self-referent phenotype matching" (also known as the "armpit effect") to compare other animals' odors to their own and to distinguish strangers from unfamiliar kin. This time she hoped to show that a different species (Spermophilus beldingi ) can "read" the chemical code of the familial scent with unprecedented precision.

Convening a squirrel family reunion in the California mountains would have been too complicated and unscientific ---- although Mateo knew exactly who was related to whom from years of placing identity tags on each new litter -- so she did the next best thing. Without harming the squirrels, she took a wide variety of scent-gland samples, transferred the scents to coded plastic cubes, and placed the cubes at the entrances of individual burrows. Mateo made sure all the odors were new to the squirrels that would smell them, so the animals could not use prior experience to evaluate the odors.

Then she waited for curious squirrels to emerge from their burrows -- and to sniff. If a squirrel spent a long time analyzing the presented scent, that would mean it had detected a less-related or totally foreign odor compound -- in keeping with a well-established standard in olfactory behavior studies. A quick sniff would be all a squirrel needed to recognize the smell of very close kin.

And that's what happened, with a few exceptions. For the most part, scents the squirrels spent the most time sniffing turned out to have come from distantly related or unrelated squirrels. The scents they recognized immediately had come from very close kin, the ones they should be helping to defend territories or evade predators. The more distant the relationships and the greater the differences in genetic make-ups, the more time was spent sniffing and evaluating scent compounds. "The sensitivity and discrimination of their olfactory apparatus is astounding," Mateo said. "They're like furry gas chromatography machines."

The squirrels' precision in kin recognition also suggests another use for scent-based discrimination, the Cornell psychologist says. Animals that tend to avoid incestuous mating with close kin (and the increased likelihood of inheriting gene-related disorders ) might analyze odors to determine whether a prospective mate is a cousin, a kissing cousin or somewhere in between. In a parallel study, also reported in the Proceedings article, Mateo found equally impressive abilities to recognize kin by odor in a non-nepotistic species, the golden-mantled ground squirrel (S. lateralis ). If the golden-mantled squirrels don't use the ability for nepotism, she wonders, might it help them avoid inbreeding in mate choice?

"When other investigators conducted scent studies and asked women to sniff T-shirts that had been worn by men either genetically similar or dissimilar to them, most women preferred the shirts worn by men with dissimilar genes," Mateo notes. "We should not be surprised to find kin-recognition abilities in a cross section of species, including humans."


Story Source:

The above story is based on materials provided by Cornell University. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.


Cite This Page:

Cornell University. "Some Animals Can Recognize Degree Of Kinship By Scent." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 26 March 2002. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2002/03/020326073053.htm>.
Cornell University. (2002, March 26). Some Animals Can Recognize Degree Of Kinship By Scent. ScienceDaily. Retrieved October 31, 2014 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2002/03/020326073053.htm
Cornell University. "Some Animals Can Recognize Degree Of Kinship By Scent." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2002/03/020326073053.htm (accessed October 31, 2014).

Share This



More Plants & Animals News

Friday, October 31, 2014

Featured Research

from universities, journals, and other organizations


Featured Videos

from AP, Reuters, AFP, and other news services

How A Chorus Led Scientists To A New Frog Species

How A Chorus Led Scientists To A New Frog Species

Newsy (Oct. 30, 2014) A frog noticed by a conservationist on New York's Staten Island has been confirmed as a new species after extensive study and genetic testing. Video provided by Newsy
Powered by NewsLook.com
Surfer Accidentally Stands on Shark, Gets Bitten

Surfer Accidentally Stands on Shark, Gets Bitten

AP (Oct. 30, 2014) A 20-year-old competition surfer said on Thursday he accidentally stepped on a shark's head before it bit him off the Australian east coast. (Oct. 30) Video provided by AP
Powered by NewsLook.com
Ebola Inflicts Heavy Toll on Guinean Potato Trade

Ebola Inflicts Heavy Toll on Guinean Potato Trade

AFP (Oct. 30, 2014) The Ebola epidemic has seen Senegal and Guinea Bissau close its borders with Guinea and the economic consequences have started to be felt, especially in Fouta Djallon, where the renowned potato industry has been hit hard. Duration: 02:01 Video provided by AFP
Powered by NewsLook.com
Genetically Altered Glowing Flower on Display in Tokyo

Genetically Altered Glowing Flower on Display in Tokyo

Reuters - Innovations Video Online (Oct. 30, 2014) Just in time for Halloween, a glowing flower goes on display in Tokyo. Instead of sorcery and magic, its creators used science to genetically modify the flower, adding a naturally fluorescent plankton protein to its genetic mix. Ben Gruber reports. Video provided by Reuters
Powered by NewsLook.com

Search ScienceDaily

Number of stories in archives: 140,361

Find with keyword(s):
Enter a keyword or phrase to search ScienceDaily for related topics and research stories.

Save/Print:
Share:

Breaking News:

Strange & Offbeat Stories


Plants & Animals

Earth & Climate

Fossils & Ruins

In Other News

... from NewsDaily.com

Science News

Health News

Environment News

Technology News



Save/Print:
Share:

Free Subscriptions


Get the latest science news with ScienceDaily's free email newsletters, updated daily and weekly. Or view hourly updated newsfeeds in your RSS reader:

Get Social & Mobile


Keep up to date with the latest news from ScienceDaily via social networks and mobile apps:

Have Feedback?


Tell us what you think of ScienceDaily -- we welcome both positive and negative comments. Have any problems using the site? Questions?
Mobile: iPhone Android Web
Follow: Facebook Twitter Google+
Subscribe: RSS Feeds Email Newsletters
Latest Headlines Health & Medicine Mind & Brain Space & Time Matter & Energy Computers & Math Plants & Animals Earth & Climate Fossils & Ruins