New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Rats Can Tell Two Languages Apart From Speech Cues, Sharing An Ability With Humans And Monkeys

Date:
January 17, 2005
Source:
American Psychological Association
Summary:
Mammals other than humans can distinguish between different speech patterns. Neuroscientists in Barcelona report that rats, like humans (newborn and adult) and Tamarin monkeys, can extract regular patterns in language from speech (prosodic) cues.
Share:
FULL STORY

WASHINGTON -- Mammals other than humans can distinguish between different speech patterns. Neuroscientists in Barcelona report that rats, like humans (newborn and adult) and Tamarin monkeys, can extract regular patterns in language from speech (prosodic) cues. The report appears in the January issue of the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, which is published by the American Psychological Association.

This study of 16 rats per each of four conditions showed that they were able to pick up enough cues from the rhythm and intonation of human speech to tell spoken Dutch from spoken Japanese. After the researchers trained rats to press a lever when hearing a synthesized five-second sentence in Dutch or Japanese, they tested the rats' response to the alternative language. Rats rewarded for responding to Japanese did not respond to Dutch and vice versa. They pressed the lever only for the language to which they'd been exposed. What's more, the rats generalized the ability to differentiate to new Dutch and new Japanese sentences they had not heard before.

This special ability to detect the features that distinguish one type of speech from another – enabled by a test using two very different spoken languages – has now been documented in three different mammalian species: Humans (both newborn and adult), Tamarin monkeys, and now rats. Scientists study Tamarin monkeys because they can use the same kinds of experiments that they use for infants, allowing for direct comparison. The rats were the first non-primate mammal studied; research on non-mammalian species (such as songbirds) may shed light on whether this ability is unique to mammals.

The rats' linguistic sophistication was limited. When experimenters used different humans to speak each sentence, the rats found it much hard to tell the languages apart. Humans, even in early infancy, can overcome this problem – and only get better at it by learning a lexicon and syntax, phonology (letter sounds), word segments, and semantic information (what words mean).

Author Juan Toro, who is about to earn his PhD, says the results were surprising. "It was striking to find that rats can track certain information that seems to be so important in language development in humans," he says. The research, he adds, shows "which abilities that humans use for language are shared with other animals, and which are uniquely human. It also suggests what sort of evolutionary precursors language might have."

Toro cautions that just because the rats share an ability with humans doesn't mean they use it the same way. He says, "Rats have not evolved the ability to track prosodic cues for linguistic requirements. It is more likely that they do it as a byproduct of other abilities that have some evolutionary relevance for them. The idea that species can use certain structures for a different function than that for which they evolved is not new. For example, human newborns coordinate all the speech information they take in to eventually make sense of language, something that a rat is not likely to do."

It is very likely, he and his co-authors suspect, that the ability to tell apart two different languages is a byproduct of more general perceptual abilities used for detecting time order through hearing – a useful adaptation for the rat. Thus, he adds, "rats can co-opt these abilities to differentiate sentences by detecting their prosodic regularities."

###

Article: "Effects of Backward Speech and Speaker Variability in Language Discrimination by Rats," Juan M. Toro, Josep B. Trobolan, PhD, and Núria Sebastián-Gallés, PhD; Grup de Recerca en Neurosciencia Cogntiva, Parc Cientific de Barcelona; Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, Vol. 31, No. 1.

(Full text of the article is available from the APA Public Affairs Office and at http://www.apa.org/releases/speech_article.pdf)

The American Psychological Association (APA), in Washington, DC, is the largest scientific and professional organization representing psychology in the United States and is the world's largest association of psychologists. APA's membership includes more than 150,000 researchers, educators, clinicians, consultants and students. Through its divisions in 53 subfields of psychology and affiliations with 60 state, territorial and Canadian provincial associations, APA works to advance psychology as a science, as a profession and as a means of promoting human welfare.


Story Source:

Materials provided by American Psychological Association. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Cite This Page:

American Psychological Association. "Rats Can Tell Two Languages Apart From Speech Cues, Sharing An Ability With Humans And Monkeys." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 17 January 2005. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2005/01/050110112619.htm>.
American Psychological Association. (2005, January 17). Rats Can Tell Two Languages Apart From Speech Cues, Sharing An Ability With Humans And Monkeys. ScienceDaily. Retrieved March 29, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2005/01/050110112619.htm
American Psychological Association. "Rats Can Tell Two Languages Apart From Speech Cues, Sharing An Ability With Humans And Monkeys." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2005/01/050110112619.htm (accessed March 29, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES