Birds form bonds that look a lot like friendship
- Date:
- May 7, 2025
- Source:
- Columbia University
- Summary:
- A study of starlings in Africa shows that they form long-term social bonds similar to human friendships.
- Share:
The fact that humans who are not related by blood help each other repeatedly over time is demonstrably true -- think of the ongoing mutual support that sustains your longest-running friendships.
But the idea that such interactions occur in the animal kingdom has been difficult to prove.
A new study of African starlings led by Alexis Earl, a former PhD student in the lab of Professor Dustin Rubenstein, and colleagues, draws on data gathered over 20 years and proves exactly that: The starlings demonstrate "reciprocity," helping each other with the expectation that the favor will eventually be returned.
"Starling societies are not just simple families, they're much more complex, containing a mixture of related and unrelated individuals that live together, much in the way that humans do," Rubenstein said.
The fact that animals help their direct blood relatives, or kin, with the goal of boosting their genetic fitness and prolonging their genes, has long been known in the scientific community. Starlings do preferentially help their relatives, but many birds also help non-relatives. Earl and colleagues discovered that this non-relative helping occurs through the formation of these reciprocal helping relationships, which sometimes take place over many years.
Proving that this type of reciprocal behavior extends to animals other than direct relatives is difficult, though, since it requires large amounts of data gathered over long stretches of time. The new paper, published this week in the journal Nature, draws on 20 years of research that Rubenstein and his colleagues have conducted on African starlings living in the harsh climate of east African savannahs. From 2002 to 2021, the researchers studied thousands of interactions between hundreds of the birds, and collected DNA from individuals in the population to examine genetic relationships. By combining 40 breeding seasons' worth of behavioral and genetic data, the team could ask questions like: Did the birds preferentially help relatives? Did they help non-relatives even when relatives were available? And did they reciprocate help with specific individuals over the years?
Ultimately, the researchers found that helpers preferentially aided relatives, but also frequently and consistently helped specific non-relative birds, even when relatives were available to help. "Many of these birds are essentially forming friendships over time," Rubenstein said. "Our next step is to explore how these relationships form, how long they last, why some relationships stay robust, while others fall apart."
The data builds on decades of research collected by Rubenstein and his colleagues and students on how and why animals socialize. They have examined animal societies not just in birds, but also in a diversity of species around the globe, including snapping shrimp throughout the Caribbean, wasps in Africa, beetles in Asia, and mice and lizards in Australia.
"I think this kind of reciprocal helping behavior is likely going on in a lot of animal societies, and people just haven't studied them long enough to be able to detect it," Rubenstein said.
Story Source:
Materials provided by Columbia University. Original written by Christopher D. Shea. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.
Journal Reference:
- Earl, A.D., Carter, G.G., Berlinger, A.G. et al. A cryptic role for reciprocal helping in a cooperatively breeding bird. Nature, 2025 DOI: 10.1038/s41586-025-08958-4
Cite This Page: