New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Expectations speed up conscious perception

Date:
February 7, 2011
Source:
Max-Planck-Gesellschaft
Summary:
The human brain works incredibly fast. However, visual impressions are so complex that their processing takes several hundred milliseconds before they enter our consciousness. Scientists have now shown that this delay may vary in length. When the brain possesses some prior information -- that is, when it already knows what it is about to see -- conscious recognition occurs faster. Until now, neuroscientists assumed that the processes leading up to conscious perception were rather rigid and that their timing did not vary.
Share:
FULL STORY

The human brain works incredibly fast. However, visual impressions are so complex that their processing takes several hundred milliseconds before they enter our consciousness. Scientists at the Max Planck Institute for Brain Research in Frankfurt am Main have now shown that this delay may vary in length. When the brain possesses some prior information − that is, when it already knows what it is about to see − conscious recognition occurs faster. Until now, neuroscientists assumed that the processes leading up to conscious perception were rather rigid and that their timing did not vary.

On their way from the eye, visual stimuli are analysed in manifold ways by different processing stages in the brain. It is not until they have passed several processing steps that the stimuli reach conscious perception. This unconscious processing prior to perception usually takes approximately 300 milliseconds. The Max Planck scientists were now able to demonstrate that the timing of this process, far from being rigid, is in fact variable. In an experiment, participants perceived stimuli more efficiently and faster if they knew what to expect.

To investigate this, the scientists showed the participants images with a background of randomly distributed dots on a monitor. During an image sequence, the distribution of the dots systematically changed such that a symbol gradually appeared. Following each image, the participants indicated if they could see the symbol by pressing a button. As soon as the symbol had appeared fully and was clearly recognisable, the scientists presented the same image sequence in reverse order, such that the symbol gradually faded again. During the entire experiment, electroencephalographic (EEG) activity of the participants was measured.

Whereas the participants took relatively long to recognise the symbol in the first sequence of images with increasing visibility, the threshold of awareness in the second, reverse presentation of images was much lower. The participants were able to recognise the letters even at very poor resolution. "Expectations based on previously acquired information apparently help to perceive the object consciously," says Lucia Melloni, first author of the study. Once the participants knew which symbol was hiding in the random field of noise, they were able to perceive it better. The scientists have thus confirmed previous studies, according to which people perceive moving objects better if they already know in which direction the objects will move.

Moreover, the measurements of EEG activity produced astonishing results. "We found that the timing of EEG activity for conscious perception changed depending on the person's expectations," says Lucia Melloni. If the participants could predict what they were going to see, the characteristic EEG pattern for conscious perception took place 100 milliseconds earlier than without prior expectations.

The scientists might thus have found a conclusive explanation for the contradictory results of other neuroscientific research groups. Depending on the study, they had sometimes found very early and sometimes very late EEG activity correlating with conscious perception. "Our research explains this variability in timing. Apparently, the brain does not process the stimuli rigidly and at the same speed; rather, it is flexible," explains Wolf Singer. Processing is thus faster if the brain only has to compare the incoming visual information with a previously established expectation. As a result, conscious perception occurs earlier. In contrast, if the brain has to assess a stimulus from scratch due to a lack of prior information, the processing takes longer.

These results may show that previous EEG studies have been interpreted incorrectly. "Since the interpretation depends heavily on the sequence of events, EEG activity may have been incorrectly allocated to consciousness processes," surmises Wolf Singer, the Director of the Department for Neurophysiology at the Max Planck Institute for Brain Research in Frankfurt. "In light of these results, it appears necessary to reinvestigate the neuronal correlates of consciousness."


Story Source:

Materials provided by Max-Planck-Gesellschaft. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. L. Melloni, C. M. Schwiedrzik, N. Muller, E. Rodriguez, W. Singer. Expectations Change the Signatures and Timing of Electrophysiological Correlates of Perceptual Awareness. Journal of Neuroscience, 2011; 31 (4): 1386 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4570-10.2011

Cite This Page:

Max-Planck-Gesellschaft. "Expectations speed up conscious perception." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 7 February 2011. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/02/110203081445.htm>.
Max-Planck-Gesellschaft. (2011, February 7). Expectations speed up conscious perception. ScienceDaily. Retrieved December 5, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/02/110203081445.htm
Max-Planck-Gesellschaft. "Expectations speed up conscious perception." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/02/110203081445.htm (accessed December 5, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES